Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring

Transcription

Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring
Chapter 16
Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single
Matriline of Ring-Tailed Lemurs (Lemur catta):
What Happened When “Grandmother” Died?
Takayo Soma and Naoki Koyama
Abstract Eviction of subordinate females is well known in female-dominated
ring-tailed lemur society. In almost all cases, evictions result from a dominant
matriline targeting aggression towards subordinate matrilines. Here, we report an
eviction in a large, single matrilineal troop after the death of the troop’s
“Grandmother.” Although the troop contained ³18 individuals and 8 females over
several years, no evictions occurred while the Grandmother was alive. After her
death, a newly dominant female and her younger sisters evicted their nieces whose
higher-ranking mother had already died. The younger relatives of the evicted
females did not follow their mother and sisters but stayed in the natal troop. We
suggest (1) the presence of certain females like Grandmother may function as a
deterrent to eviction among descendants; (2) if eviction occurs within a single
matriline, aggressors evict the most distant kin; (3) a newly dominant female may
form a subgroup with her younger and subordinate sisters rather than older and
higher-ranking ones to circumvent intragroup competition; and (4) the juveniles of
evicted female kin do not always follow the nomadic evictees but may choose the
safer strategy of remaining within their natal troop.
Resume L’éviction de femelles subordonnées dans des groupes sociaux dominés
par les femelles est bien connue chez le Lémur catta. Dans presque tous les cas, les
évictions font suite à des agressions ciblées dirigées d’une lignée dominante vers
une lignée subordonnée. Nous rapportons ici un cas d’éviction dans une troupe
constituée d’une seule lignée matrilinéaire qui a fait suite à la mort de la « Grandmère ». Bien que la troupe contienne ³ 18 individus et 8 femelles depuis plusieurs
années, aucun cas d’éviction n’a été observé tant que Grand-mère était en vie. Après
sa mort, une nouvelle femelle dominante et ses jeunes sœurs ont évincé leurs nièces
dont la mère, de rang élevé, était morte auparavant. Les jeunes apparentés aux
T. Soma (*) • N. Koyama
The Center for African Area Studies, Kyoto University,
46 Shimoadachi-cho, Yoshida, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan
e-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]
J. Masters et al. (eds.), Leaping Ahead: Advances in Prosimian Biology, Developments
in Primatology: Progress and Prospects, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-4511-1_16,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
137
138
T. Soma and N. Koyama
femelles évincées n’ont pas suivi leurs mères et sœurs mais sont restés dans la
troupe natale. Nous suggérons que (1) la présence de certaines femelles telles que
Grand-mère pourrait inhiber les évictions entre ses descendants; (2) quand une éviction se produit au sein d’un groupe matrilinéaire, les agresseurs évincent les individus qui leur sont le moins apparentés; (3) une nouvelle femelle dominante pourrait
former un sous-groupe composé de sœurs plus jeunes qui leur sont subordonnées,
plutôt que de femelles de haut rang et plus âgées, afin de diminuer la compétition
entre groupes; (4) les jeunes apparentés aux femelles évincées ne suivent pas toujours les femelles évincées, mais peuvent choisir la stratégie plus sûre consistant à
demeurer dans leur groupe natal.
Introduction
Female ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) are dominant over males (Jolly 1966;
Kappeler 1990; Sauther 1993). Eviction of females from their natal troops has
been observed in troops with ³16 individuals, at least 7–8 of whom are adult
females (Jolly et al. 2002; Ichino and Koyama 2006), as a result of targeting
aggression (intense and persistent aggression from one or more females to one or
more subordinate individuals; Vick and Pereira 1989; Koyama et al. 2002; Ichino
and Koyama 2006).
Almost all previous descriptions of female eviction have involved aggression by
dominant kin groups towards subordinate ones, suggesting that female eviction and
troop fission occur between matrilineal groups to reduce intratroop competition
(Koyama et al. 2002; Ichino and Koyama 2006). We report a sequence of observations from targeting aggression and female eviction to troop fission in a single
matriline after the death of their “Grandmother.” In our terminology, troops have at
least one adult male while groups have no adult males (Koyama 1991). The terms
nomadic troop and nomadic group describe troops/groups that do not maintain
exclusive core areas (Jolly and Pride 1999). Troop fission refers to the separation of
troop members into permanent new social troops with secure ranges (Vick and
Pereira 1989; Hood and Jolly 1995; Koyama et al. 2002).
Study Site and Troop History
Our study was conducted in the Berenty Private Reserve adjacent to the Mandrare
River in southern Madagascar (see Chap. 39). The reserve is approximate 240 ha,
and composed of gallery forest, scrub forest, and spiny forest (Jolly 1966; Jolly and
Pride 1999). Individuals in the seven study troops have been systematically identified
and named by Koyama and his colleagues since 1989 (Chap. 42). Every female has
her own code name; e.g., ME89♀ refers to the daughter born to mother ME♀ in the
year 1989 and ME8993♀ is the granddaughter of ME♀ born to ME89♀ in 1993.
16
Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring-Tailed...
139
The composition of C1 troop over 2 years (2004 and 2005) is shown in Table 16.1.
Individuals ³3 years of age were considered adult. Rank was determined by submissive behaviors like spat calls. C1 troop split from the original C troop in 1989, sharing its original core area with the neighboring CX troop. When observations
commenced in September 2004, C1 comprised 21 individuals including 8 adult
females and 5 adult males. Their territory, which they defended in confrontations
with other troops, was 4.19 ha, situated in the gallery forest, and included the tourist
bungalow area. A map showing the territories of these and adjacent troops can be
found in Koyama et al. (2006) and in Chap. 42 (Fig. 42.4).
Methods
The study formed part of a broader investigation into feeding ecology that spanned
13 months (September 16, 2004–February 15, 2005; April 10–November 20, 2005)
and focused on two ring-tailed lemur troops, C1 and CX. Observations were conducted from 6:00 to 18:00 in the dry season and from 5:00 to 19:00 in the rainy
season. We used focal animal sampling (Altmann 1974) and 5-min instantaneous
point sampling to collect data on proximity of individuals (³2 m). When targeting
aggression, eviction or other special behaviors occurred, and we collected data ad
libitum, including time, behavior, and place.
Results
We observed the following sequence of events.
1. Rank change and emergence of subgroups before September 2004
Troop C1 was composed of the descendants of one female, “Grandmother”
ME89♀. She gave birth to 11 offspring, 8 of whom were still living at the time
of the study (6 females and 2 males). From September 2000 to 2006, the troop
contained 18–21 individuals and 6–8 adult females. The female dominance hierarchy generally reflected birth order, with the older daughters being more dominant. In September 2003, ME89♀’s fourth daughter ME8998♀ became the
second-ranked female after her mother. ME89♀ was not very active but still
dominant. Both ME89♀ and her second daughter ME8994♀ died sometime
between December 2003 and August 2004.
By September 2004, C1 troop contained 21 individuals (Table 16.1). After the
death of Grandmother ME89♀, ME8998♀ (6 years old) became the Alpha
female. She and her three younger sisters without offspring, ME8900♀,
ME8901♀, and ME8999♀, formed a subgroup (proximity rate = 21.6, 18.4, and
10.6 % respectively, N = 2 days). The oldest sister (ME8993♀) was Beta-ranked
and associated with her two sons, ME899302♂ and ME899303♂ (24.7 and
36.1 %, N = 2 days). Older, middle-ranked ME8997♀ associated often with her
ME899402♀
ME899403♀
ME899804♀
Sad
J
I
0
1
2
6
11
7
4
3
5
3
5
Ad: Adult, Sad: Subadult, J: Juvenile, I:Infant
Troop C1 = 21 + 4NI
Adult females N = 8
→:Emigration
↓:Immigration
ME8998♀
ME8993♀
ME8997♀
ME8900♀
ME8901♀
ME899499♀
ME899401♀
ME8999♀
Ad
ME899702♂
ME899302♂
ME8902♂
ME8903♂
ME89949903♂
ME899303♂
ME890004♂
ME890104♂
ME89949904♂
TUR♂
SEN♂
FUJ♂
GRZ ♂→
KTN♂
Male
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
Age
Order
Female
(a) Names of individuals (September 2004)
Class
Rank
2
2
2
1
1
1
0
0
0
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
Age
Table 16.1 Age–sex composition and names of individuals in C1 troop over the study
ME899403♀
ME8998♀
ME8997♀
ME8901♀
ME8900♀
ME8993♀
ME8999♀
Troop C1 = 21 + 3NI
Adult females N = 6
Evicted
ME899499♀
ME899401♀
I
J
Sad
Ad
Female
6
4
2
7
8
4
5
12
6
Age
Left C1 troop
ME899402♀
ME890005♂
ME890105♂
ME899805♂
TUR♂
SEN♂
FUJ♂
↓GRI♂
↓FTM♂
ME8902♂
ME899702♂
ME899302♂
KTN♂
↓TUN♂
ME8903♂
ME899303♂
ME89949903♂
ME890004♂
Male
(b) Names of individuals (September 2005)
Class
3
0
0
0
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
Unknown
3
3
3
Unknown
Unknown
2
2
2
1
Age
140
T. Soma and N. Koyama
16
Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring-Tailed...
141
Fig. 16.1 Diagram of social changes in the C1 troop before and after the evictions
subadult son, ME899702♂. ME8994♀’s daughters, ME899499♀, ME899401♀,
ME899402♀, and ME899403♀, who had not stayed in the troop center near the
Alpha female, kept their distance from the other females on September 17.
ME899499♀ had also not remained near the Alpha (0 %) and other females
(2.8–6.8 %) but stayed with her sisters (36.8 %) on October 18. Thus, there were
four female subgroups in September 2004 (1) the Alpha female and her younger
sisters, (2) the older sister family, (3) the oldest sister family, and (4) the niece
family (Fig. 16.1).
2. Targeting aggression and eviction, late September to early October 2004
The Alpha female (ME8998♀) and ME8900♀ began targeting aggression at
ME8994♀’s adult daughters in mid-September 2004 (5 times in 3 days).
ME8998♀, ME8900♀, ME8999♀ (lost on September 18), and ME899401♀ had
infants during this period. At 15:00 on October 2, ME899499♀ and ME899401♀
were chased by the Alpha subgroup and ran away, crossing the territories of two
other troops into A2 territory. After this eviction, whenever C1 troop encountered ME8994♀’s adult daughters, three of the Alpha subgroup (ME8998♀,
ME8900♀, and ME8901♀) pursued their adult nieces aggressively, while
ME8993♀, ME8997♀, and ME8999♀ did not.
The older ME8997♀ began to participate in the Alpha female subgroup after
the eviction. ME8993♀, the oldest sister, fell in rank from Beta to second lowest
female. Subordinate ME8999♀ left the Alpha female group and moved with
ME8993♀. The new subgroups were (1) the dominant group, (2) the subordinate
group, and (3) the adult nieces outside the troop (Fig. 16.1). The subordinate
142
T. Soma and N. Koyama
group was not aggressive to ME899499♀ and ME899401♀ when they encountered each other on the C1 boundary.
The second daughter of ME8994♀, ME899401♀, had a male infant when the
eviction occurred. The infant fell from his mother and was found in a weakened
condition near the boundary of the neighboring YF troop on October 3. The
dominant group was not aggressive towards the immature ME899402♀ and
ME899403♀, and even though their older sisters had been chased, they were
allowed to return to C1 troop. ME899402♀ sometimes stayed with her sisters but
most often returned to C1 troop. However, she joined her older sisters on October
15 and remained with them.
3. Juveniles remained in their natal troop, early October
There were two juveniles in the evicted niece family. The youngest, ME899403♀,
tried to follow her sisters, but when ME899499♀ and ME899401♀ fled, she
could not keep up and lost track of them. On October 15, when the C1 troop
encountered ME899499♀ and ME899401♀ at the A2/C1 boundary, they were
attacked by the Alpha female; ME899402♀ followed her sisters but ME899403♀
could not, emitting loud “lost calls,” and ceased following her sisters from that
time. ME899499♀’s son, ME89949903♂, did not follow his mother but stayed
in C1 troop. He spent time with his mother outside the C1 territory three times,
when he groomed and moved with his family, but returned to C1 in the evening.
Although he became less active and lost weight and condition, he survived and
was not subjected to aggression by troop members.
4. Formation of a nomadic troop after the mating season, April 2005
From November to December 2004, the adult nieces avoided the C1 home range,
although they were seen in the neighboring CX range. However, from the end of
January 2005, when the top-ranking food plant Azadirachta indica was fruiting,
they entered the C1 range several times quietly to feed on fruits. Whenever they
were discovered by the dominant group, they were chased vehemently.
In the April 2005 mating season, we frequently found the adult nieces near the
C1/A2 boundary with two accompanying males (5 times in 9 days). One A2
male joined them temporarily during the mating season, but another 6-year-old
male, CW9094♂ from the neighboring CX troop, remained with them beyond
this period. Hence, the adult nieces formed a new troop (Koyama 1991). The
dominant group continued to chase them, but the adult nieces resisted their
aggression, fighting back on the confrontation line. ME8993♀ and ME8999♀
did not attack the nieces in the absence of the dominant group members. Although
they were still nomadic (Ichino and Koyama 2006), they intruded frequently
into the C1, A2, and CX home ranges, from April through the dry season to
November.
By the September 2005 birth season, the adult nieces had gained a sleeping
site on the A1/A2 boundary. However, they did not often win confrontations with
other troops, and ME899499♀ dropped her infant when they retreated on October
11. They did not succeed in establishing a territory. On October 31, 2005, their
male consort (CW909499♂) was found dead from unidentified causes, and the
adult nieces again became a nomadic group.
16
Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring-Tailed...
143
Discussion
Troop Hierarchy Change After the Death of Grandmother
Almost all cases of eviction described previously at Berenty and Beza Mahafaly
were carried out by dominant matrilineal groups against subordinate matrilines
(Koyama 1991; Hood and Jolly 1995; Jolly and Pride 1999; Gould et al. 2003;
Ichino and Koyama 2006). In the T2 troop, a similar classic eviction occurred in
September 2003 (N = 18, 8 adult females comprised of 6 dominant kin and 2 subordinate kin, 5 adult males, dominant kin group = 11, subordinate kin group = 3).
Ichino and Koyama (2006) proposed that female eviction occurred when the troop
contained ³16 individuals (mean = 22.5, range 16–28) and the number of adult
females was ³7 (mean = 8.4, range 7–10), while Jolly et al. (2002) favored ³8
females. When C1 contained more than two matrilineal groups in 2000, ME89♀
and her daughters evicted another kin group.
In C2A troop, two females were evicted in October 2009 when the number of
adult females reached 7; the Alpha female, her daughters, and granddaughter evicted
her cousin’s daughters. In the A2 troop, which lacked a Grandmother figure, 3 adult
females were evicted in October 2012 when the adult female number was 10 (our
observations).
In the case we have described, all the females in C1 troop were daughters or
granddaughters of Grandmother ME89♀, who retained the Alpha rank until she
died. Although C1 troop comprised more than 16 individuals and 8 females, there
were no evictions and the dominance hierarchy was stable. After Grandmother’s
death, hierarchy stability crumbled and targeting aggressions led to evictions. The
presence of an old matriarch thus may deter evictions when all the troop’s females
are her descendants.
Choice of Eviction Targets and Allies
If a dominant female evicts other females within the same matriline to avoid food
competition, nieces would be more appropriate targets than sisters because of their
more distant relationship; this was the case in our study.
ME8998♀ initially formed a subgroup with her younger sisters rather than the
older, stronger ones with offspring. Among cercopithecine monkeys, adult sisters
usually rank in inverse order of age (Japanese macaque: Koyama 1970; Nakamichi
et al. 1995; long-tailed macaque: Netto and van Hooff 1986), female dominance
ranks remain stable over considerable periods, and daughters acquire ranks immediately below those of their mothers (Nakamichi et al. 1995). In ring-tailed lemurs,
however, daughters are not invariably ranked immediately below their mothers, and
adult sisters are dominant over younger sisters (Nakamichi and Koyama 1997).
Moreover, dominance relations change frequently in both sexes (Jolly 1966; Taylor
144
T. Soma and N. Koyama
and Sussman 1985; Nakamichi et al. 1997) and are largely determined by attributes
like age, size, fighting ability, and social skills (Nakamichi and Koyama 1997).
When ME8998♀ became an adult, her older sisters, ME8993♀ and ME8994♀,
were higher ranking. ME8998♀ emitted submissive “spat” calls and was displaced
by them, while her younger sisters were not aggressive towards her. She chose to
form a subgroup with her more compliant younger sisters in intragroup contests,
ultimately depriving her older sisters of their high ranks. Moreover, targeting her
nieces for eviction could increase ME8998♀’s inclusive fitness.
Juvenile Strategies to Cope with the Eviction of Close Kin
Although their close kin, including their mothers, were evicted, the juveniles chose
to stay in C1 troop. They groomed and moved with their mothers and sisters for a
few hours when they met them, indicating they did not forget their close kin.
When a female of CX troop died, her female juvenile did not have any kin with
whom to groom and huddle. She did not feed well and died 2 weeks after the death
of her mother (Soma 2006). ME89♀’s last offspring, ME8903♂, also became markedly unhealthy after his mother’s death. However, ME899403♀ and ME899403♂
had juvenile (N = 2) and subadult males (N = 3) and adult females from the same
matriline with whom they could groom and huddle, and the dominant subgroup
members were not especially aggressive towards them. The adult females were tolerant and groomed the juveniles. Later, when ME8990♀ was evicted by the returned
adult nieces in 2006, her son ME899005♂ did not follow his mother and remained
in C1 troop.
Juveniles face potential costs in following nomadic kin as opposed to staying in
their natal troops. They might not be able to follow the adults when they intrude into
another territory and are chased by the proprietors. Juveniles left alone in unfamiliar
territories are vulnerable to predation, and October may be a time of especially high
vulnerability for infant ring-tailed lemurs, because this period coincides with the
nesting season of both harrier hawks and buzzards, which have increased nutritional
needs for egg laying and incubation (Sauther 1989). A subadult male of the T1B
troop was preyed upon by a Madagascar buzzard, Buteo brachypterus, in September
2003 (Jolly and Soma, personal observation). In May 2005, a male juvenile of CX
troop (N = 10, 5 adult females and 3 adult males) was eaten by a feral dog, even
though he was moving with his mother in the troop (Soma, personal observation).
Juveniles may be less vulnerable than infants but still face danger moving alone,
making it safer for them to stay in the natal troop with its enhanced capacity for
predator detection.
When C1 and CX troops split, Koyama et al. (2002) found the juveniles initially
stayed in the natal C1 troop and did not follow their mother and sisters. However,
once their kin succeeded in establishing a territory, the juveniles joined their kin.
Hence, ME899403♀ and ME89949903♂ may join the adult nieces’ troop when
they establish a stable territory.
16
Eviction and Troop Reconstruction in a Single Matriline of Ring-Tailed...
145
Conclusions
1. The existence of a matriarch like Grandmother in a ring-tailed lemur troop may
deter evictions when all the troop’s females are her descendants.
2. If eviction occurs within a single matriline, targeting the most distantly related
kin could increase the inclusive fitness of the dominant and high-ranking
antagonists.
3. The dominant female may form a subgroup with her younger and subordinate
sisters rather than the older and higher-ranking ones to circumvent intragroup
competition.
4. Juveniles of evicted females may not follow their closest nomadic kin, preferring
the relative safety of their natal troops.
Acknowledgments We thank Jean de Healme and family for their hospitality and for allowing us
to conduct research in Berenty Reserve, and the Government of Madagascar and the Botanical and
Zoological Park of Tsimbazaza for research permission. TS is grateful to Alison Jolly for advice
and encouragement.
References
Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267
Gould L, Sussman RW, Sauther ML (2003) Demographic and life-history patterns in a population
of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at Beza Mahafaly Reserve, Madagascar: a 15-year perspective. Am J Phys Anthropol 120:182–194
Hood LC, Jolly A (1995) Troop fission in female Lemur catta at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. Int
J Primatol 16:997–1015
Ichino S, Koyama N (2006) Social change in a wild population of ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta)
at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H (eds)
Ringtailed lemur biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar. Springer, New York, pp 233–244
Jolly A (1966) Lemur behavior. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Jolly A, Pride Y (1999) Troop histories and range inertia of Lemur catta at Berenty, Madagascar: a
33-year perspective. Int J Primatol 20:359–373
Jolly A, Dobson A, Rasamimanana HM, Walker J, O’Conner S, Solberg M, Perel V (2002)
Demography of Lemur catta at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar: effects of troop size, habitat and
rainfall. Int J Primatol 23:327–353
Kappeler PM (1990) Female dominance in Lemur catta: more than just female feeding priority?
Folia Primatol 55:92–95
Koyama N (1970) Changes in dominance rank and division of a wild Japanese monkey troop in
Arashiyama. Primates 11:335–390
Koyama N (1991) Troop division and inter-troop relationships of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta)
at Berenty, Madagascar. In: Ehara A, Kimura T, Takenaka O, Iwamoto M (eds) Primatology
today. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 173–176
Koyama N, Nakamichi N, Ichino S, Takahata Y (2002) Population and social dynamics changes in
ring-tailed lemur troops at Berenty, Madagascar between 1989–1999. Primates 43:291–314
Koyama N, Soma T, Ichino S, Takahata Y (2006) Home ranges of ringtailed emur troops and the
density of large trees at Berenty reserve, Madagascar. In: Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N,
Rasamimanana H (eds) Ringtailed lemur biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar. Springer,
New York, pp 86–101
146
T. Soma and N. Koyama
Nakamichi M, Itoigawa N, Imakawa S, Machida S (1995) Behavioral characteristics of old female
Japanese monkeys in a free-ranging group of Japanese monkeys at Katsurayama. Primates
25:192–203
Nakamichi M, Rakototiana MLO, Koyama N (1997) Effects of spatial proximity and alliances on
dominance relations among female ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) at the Berenty Reserve,
Madagascar. Primates 38:331–340
Nakamichi M, Koyama N (1997) Social relationships among ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) in
two free-ranging troops at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 18:73–93
Netto WJ, van Hooff JARAM (1986) Conflict interference and the development of dominance
relationships in immature Macaca fasicularis. In: Else JG, Lee PC (eds) Primate ontogeny,
cognition and social behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 291–300
Sauther ML (1989) Antipredator behavior in troops of free-ranging Lemur catta at Beza Mahafaly
Special Reserve, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 10:595–606
Sauther ML (1993) Resource competition in wild populations of ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta):
implications for female dominance. In: Kappeler PM, Ganzhorn JU (eds) Lemur social systems
and their ecological basis. Plenum, New York, pp 135–152
Soma T (2006) Tradition and novelty: Lemur catta feeding strategy on introduced tree species at
Berenty Reserve. In: Jolly A, Sussman RW, Koyama N, Rasamimanana H (eds) Ringtailed
lemur biology: Lemur catta in Madagascar. Springer, New York, pp 141–159
Taylor L, Sussman RW (1985) A preliminary study of kinship and social organization in a
semi-free-ranging group of Lemur catta. Int J Primatol 6:601–614
Vick GL, Pereira ME (1989) Episodic targeting aggression and the histories of Lemur catta social
groups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 25:3–12

Documents pareils